Abstract
The intuitive experience of dreaming is that we “see” something with our mind’s eye and react to it. Indeed, dream formation conceptualizations have often focused on the visual image-to-verbal thought direction of causality, inspired by bottom-up models of perception. However, despite being experienced as external to us, the dream environment is internally generated, meaning that dreams are more like imagination than perception. Dream thoughts (experienced with a sense of agency or selfhood) also affect subsequent dream images (experienced as non-agentive or external to one’s self). Thus, oneiric experiences are made up of reciprocal, continuous, moment-to-moment influences between the dream and the dreamer: visual images and verbal, abstract, or otherwise imageless thoughts. During wakefulness, a dreamlike cognitive processing dynamic of ever-changing meaningful image-thought-image momentary associations coupled with reduced agentive control of images may be present to a greater or lesser extent. Non-agentive visual images as a significant driving force in one’s spontaneous mental activity may relate to individual differences in fantasy proneness, suggestibility, dissociative absorption, or imagination tendencies. This feature carries implications for the field of spontaneous thought. Off-task thinking, or mind-wandering, are non-specific labels that may be used to denote internally generated mentation. Considering the tendency for dreamlike associations may represent a specific distinction, which may be relevant for understanding psychopathology. Whereas some individuals may find themselves distracted by abstract or semantic associations (e.g., verbal self-derogatory inner speech), others may find themselves drawn to intrusive—or alluring—visual images, which may interact significantly with their verbal thoughts and associations and even be experienced with decreased sense of agency. This may occur in obsessive-compulsive disorder, dissociative disorders, or maladaptive daydreaming, suggesting that clinicians need to pay attention to their clients’ spontaneous visual imagery. If a picture is worth a thousand words and seeing is believing, psychologists should be curious of how a visual thinking style may impact mental health.
References
Aardema, F., & O’Connor, K. (2003). Seeing white bears that are not there: Inference processes in obsessions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 17(1), 23–37. https://doi.org/10.1891/jcop.17.1.23.58270
Aardema, F., O’Connor, K. P., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., Marchand, A., & Todorov, C. (2005). Inferential confusion in obsessive–compulsive disorder: The inferential confusion questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(3), 293–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.003
Aardema, F., & Wu, K. D. (2011). Imaginative, dissociative, and schizotypal processes in obsessive‐compulsive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(1), 74–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20729
Agargun, M. Y., Kara, H., Özer, Ö. A., Selvi, Y., Kiran, Ü., & Özer, B. (2003). Clinical importance of nightmare disorder in patients with dissociative disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 57, 575–579. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01169.x
Alfasi, D., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Does alexithymia moderate the relation between stress and general sleep experiences? Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 87–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.014
Arntz, A. (2012). Imagery rescripting as a therapeutic technique: Review of clinical trials, basic studies, and research agenda. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(2), 189–208. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.024211
Aviram, L., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Lucid dreaming: Intensity, but not frequency, is inversely related to psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 384. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00384
Barrett, D. (Ed.). (1996). Dreams in multiple personality disorder. In Trauma and dreams (pp. 68–81). Harvard University Press.
Beaulieu-Prévost, D., Charneau Simard, C., & Zadra, A. (2009). Making sense of dream experiences: A multidimensional approach to beliefs about dreams. Dreaming, 19(3), 119–134. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017279
Bigelsen, J., Lehrfeld, J. M., Jopp, D. S., & Somer, E. (2016). Maladaptive daydreaming: Evidence for an under-researched mental health disorder. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 254–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.017
Bigelsen, J., & Schupak, C. (2011). Compulsive fantasy: Proposed evidence of an under-reported syndrome through a systematic study of 90 self-identified non-normative fantasizers. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1634–1648. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.013
Blagrove, M. (2011). Distinguishing continuity/discontinuity, function, and insight when investigating dream content. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 45–47. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9153
Blagrove, M., & Hartnell, S. J. (2000). Lucid dreaming: Associations with internal locus of control, need for cognition and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(1), 41–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00078-1
Boterhoven de Haan, K., Lee, C., Fassbinder, E., Van Es, S., Menninga, S., Meewisse, M., Rijkeboer, M., Kousemaker, M., & Arntz, A. (2020). Imagery rescripting and eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing as treatment for adults with post-traumatic stress disorder from childhood trauma: Randomised clinical trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 217(5), 609–615. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.158
Bouvard, M., Fournet, N., Denis, A., Sixdenier, A., & Clark, D. (2017). Intrusive thoughts in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical participants: A comparison using the International Intrusive Thought Interview Schedule. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 46(4), 287–299. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2016.1262894
Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesenten, N. J., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Selbie, S., Belenky, G., & Herscovitch, P. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle: An H2(15)o PET study. Brain, 120(7), 1173–1197. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.7.1173
Bréchet, L., Brunet, D., Perogamvros, L., Tononi, G., & Michel, C. M. (2020). EEG microstates of dreams. Scientific Reports, 10, 17069. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-74075-z
Bregman-Hai, N., Abitbul-Gordon, S., Deutsch, I., Garbi, D., Shelef, L., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Leave everything to the imagination: Cognitive functioning of individuals high in dissociative absorption and imaginative involvement. Journal of Research in Personality, 76, 129–138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.08.004
Bregman-Hai, N., Kessler, Y., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2020). Who wrote that? Automaticity and reduced sense of agency in individuals prone to dissociative absorption. Consciousness & Cognition, 78, 102861. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.102861
Bulkeley, K., & Domhoff, G. W. (2010). Detecting meaning in dream reports: An extension of a word search approach. Dreaming, 20(2), 77–95. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019773
Carlson, E. B., & Putnam, F. W. (1993). An update on the dissociative experiences scale. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 6(1), 16–27. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1994-27927-001
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17, 718–731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Christoff, K., Mills, C., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Irving, Z. C., Thompson, E., Fox, K. C. R., & Kam, J. W. Y. (2018). Mind-wandering as a scientific concept: Cutting through the definitional haze. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 957–959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.004
Cicogna, P., & Bosinelli, M. (2001). Consciousness during dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(1), 26–41. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.2000.0471
Crossman, A. M., Scullin, M. H., & Melnyk, L. (2004). Individual and developmental differences in suggestibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(8), 941–945. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1079
D’Argembeau, A., & Linden, M. van der. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.001
Dang-Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., Petit, D., Mazza, S., Montplaisir, J., & Maquet, P. (2007). Neuroimaging in sleep medicine. Sleep Medicine, 8(4), 349–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.006
Dennett, D. C. (1976). Are dreams experiences? The Philosophical Review, 85, 151–171. https://doi.org/10.2307/2183728
Dentico, D., Cheung, B. L., Chang, J. Y., Guokas, J., Boly, M., Tononi, G., & Van Veen, B. (2014). Reversal of cortical information flow during visual imagery as compared to visual perception. Neuroimage, 100, 237–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.081
Domhoff, G. W. (2001). A new neurocognitive theory of dreams. Dreaming, 11(1), 13–33. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009464416649
Domhoff, G. W. (2011a). Dreams are embodied simulations that dramatize conceptions and concerns: The continuity hypothesis in empirical, theoretical, and historical context. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 50–62. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9137
Domhoff, G. W. (2011b). The neural substrate for dreaming: Is it a subsystem of the default network? Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1163–1174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.001
Domhoff, G. W. (2017). The invasion of the concept snatchers: The origins, distortions, and future of the continuity hypothesis. Dreaming, 27(1), 14–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000047
Edwards, D. J. A. (2007). Restructuring implicational meaning through memory based imagery: Some historical notes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 306–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.001
Erdelyi, M. H. (2017). The continuity hypothesis. Dreaming, 27(4), 334–344. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000063
Falk, D. R., & Hill, C. E. (1995). The effectiveness of dream interpretation groups for women undergoing a divorce transition. Dreaming, 5(1), 29–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094421
Ferrante, E., Marino, A., Guglielmucci, F., & Schimmenti, A. (2022). The mediating role of dissociation and shame in the relationship between emotional trauma and maladaptive daydreaming. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9(1), 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000253
Foulkes, D., & Domhoff, G. W. (2014). Bottom-up or top-down in dream neuroscience? A top-down critique of two bottom-up studies. Consciousness & Cognition, 27, 168–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.05.002
Freud, S. (1900). Die traumdeutung. Franz Deuticke.
Friston, K. (2018). Does predictive coding have a future? Nature Neuroscience, 21, 1019–1021. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0200-7
Gennaro, A., Kipp, S., Viol, K., Felice, G., Andreassi, S., Aichhorn, W., S, S., & Schiepek, G. (2020). A phase transition of the unconscious: Automated text analysis of dreams in psychoanalytic psychotherapy. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01667
Giesbrecht, T., Smeets, T., Leppink, J., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2007). Acute dissociation after 1 night of sleep loss. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 599–606. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.599
Gravitz, M. A. (1994). Memory reconstruction by hypnosis as a therapeutic technique. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 31(4), 687–691. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.31.4.687
Hall, C. (1953). A cognitive theory of dream symbols. Journal of General Psychology, 48, 169–186. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1953.9920189
Harb, G. C., Brownlow, J. A., & Ross, R. J. (2016). Posttraumatic nightmares and imagery rehearsal: The possible role of lucid dreaming. Dreaming, 26, 238–249. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000030
Hartmann, E. (1996). Outline for a theory on the nature and functions of dreaming. Dreaming, 6, 147–170. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094452
Hartmann, E. (1998). Nightmare after trauma as paradigm for all dreams: A new approach to the nature and functions of dreaming. Psychiatry, 61(3), 223–238. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1998.11024834
Hartmann, E., & Kunzendorf, R. G. (2006). Boundaries and dreams. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 26(1), 101–115. https://doi.org/10.2190/HK76-038K-407M-8670
Hegdé, J., & Felleman, D. J. (2007). Reappraising the functional implications of the primate visual anatomical hierarchy. Neuroscientist, 13(5), 416–421. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858407305201
Hill, C. E., & Goates, M. K. (2004). Research on the hill cognitive-experiential dream model. In C. E. Hill (Ed.), Dream work in therapy: Facilitating exploration, insight, and action (pp. 245–288). American Psychological Association.
Hill, C. E., Liu, J., Spangler, P., Sim, W., & Schottenbauer, M. (2008). Working with dreams in psychotherapy: What do psychoanalytic therapists report that they do? Psychoanalytic Psychology, 25(4), 565–573. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013539
Hobson, A. J., & Pace-Schott, E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: Neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 679–693. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn915
Hobson, J. A., Hong, C. C.-H., & Friston, K. J. (2014). Virtual reality and consciousness inference in dreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1133. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01133
Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 134(12), 1335–1348. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335
Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, F. E., & Stickgold, R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 793–842. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00003976
Hobson, J. A., & Schredl, M. (2011). The continuity and discontinuity between waking and dreaming: A dialogue between michael schredl and allan hobson concerning the adequacy and completeness of these notions. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(1), 3–7. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.1.9087
Horton, C. L., & Malinowski, J. E. (2015). Autobiographical memory and hyperassociativity in the dreaming brain: Implications for memory consolidation in sleep. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 874. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00874
Hurlburt, R. T., Heavey, C. L., & Kelsey, J. M. (2013). Toward a phenomenology of inner speaking. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1477–1494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.10.003
Hurlburt, R. T., Heavey, C. L., Lapping-Carr, L., Krumm, A. E., Moynihan, S. A., Kaneshiro, C., Brouwers, V. P., Turner, D. K., & Kelsey, J. M. (2022). Measuring the frequency of inner-experience characteristics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(2), 559–571. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621990379
Iachini, T., Maffei, L., Masullo, M., Senese, V. P., Rapuano, M., Pascale, A., Sorrentino, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2019). The experience of virtual reality: Are individual differences in mental imagery associated with sense of presence? Cognitive Processing, 20, 291–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-018-0897-y
Isaac, A. R., & Marks, D. F. (1994). Individual differences in mental imagery experience: Developmental changes and specialization. British Journal of Psychology, 85(4), 479–500. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02536.x
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Macmillan & Co.
Jopp, D. S., Dupuis, M., Somer, E., Hagani, N., & Herscu, O. (2019). Validation of the hebrew version of the maladaptive daydreaming scale (MDS-h): Evidence for a generalizable measure of pathological daydreaming. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(3), 242–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000162
Kahan, T. L., & LaBerge, S. P. (2011). Dreaming and waking: Similarities and differences revisited. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 494–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.002
Koffel, E., & Watson, D. (2009). Unusual sleep experiences, dissociation, and schizotypy: Evidence for a common domain. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 548–559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.004
Konkoly, K., & Burke, C. T. (2019). Can learning to lucid dream promote personal growth? Dreaming, 29(2), 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000101
Kozmová, M., & Wolman, R. N. (2006). Self-awareness in dreaming. Dreaming, 16(3), 196–214. https://doi.org/10.1037/1053-0797.16.3.196
Krakow, B., & Zadra, A. (2010). Imagery rehearsal therapy: Principles and practice. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 5(2), 289–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsmc.2010.01.004
Krantz, S., & Hammen, C. L. (1979). Assessment of cognitive bias in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88(6), 611–619. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.88.6.611
Kunzendorf, R. G., Hartmann, E., Cohen, R., & Cutler, J. (1997). Bizarreness of the dreams and daydreams reported by individuals with thin and thick boundaries. Dreaming, 7(4), 265–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094482
LaBerge, S. P. (1985). Lucid dreaming. Tarcher.
Leavitt, F. (1999). Dissociative experiences scale taxon and measurement of dissociative pathology: Does the taxon add to an understanding of dissociation and its associated pathologies? Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 6, 427–440. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026275916184
Leavitt, F. (2001). MMPI profile characteristics of women with varying levels of normal dissociation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(12), 1469–1477. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.1110
Lechner-Meichsner, F., Ehring, T., Kruger-Gottschalk, A., Morina, N., Plankl, C., & Steil, R. (2023). Using imagery rescripting to treat posttraumatic stress disorder in refugees: A case study. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 30(4), 646–656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2022.06.002
Levin, R., & Nielsen, T. (2009). Nightmares, bad dreams, and emotion dysregulation: A review and new neurocognitive model of dreaming. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 84–88. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01614.x
Lipton, M. G., Brewin, C. R., Linke, S., & Halperin, J. (2010). Distinguishing features of intrusive images in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(8), 816–822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.003
Llewellyn, S. (2013). Such stuff as dreams are made on? Elaborative encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampus. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 589–607. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12003135
Llewellyn, S. (2015). Dream to predict? REM dreaming as prospective coding. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01961
Malinowski, J. E. (2015). Dreaming and personality: Wake-dream continuity, thought suppression, and the big five inventory. Consciousness and Cognition, 38, 9–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.10.004
Malinowski, J. E., Fylan, F., & Horton, C. L. (2014). Experiencing “continuity”: A qualitative investigation of waking life in dreams. Dreaming, 24(3), 161–175. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037305
Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2014a). Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life experiences into dreams. Dreaming, 24(1), 18–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036017
Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2014b). Memory sources of dreams: The incorporation of autobiographical rather than episodic experiences. Journal of Sleep Research, 23(4), 441–447. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12134
Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2015). Metaphor and hyperassociativity: The imagination mechanisms behind emotion assimilation in sleep and dreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1132. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01132
Margherita, G., Caffieri, A., Mariani, R., Filosa, M., Manari, T., Lenzo, V., Quattropani, M. C., Vegni, E., Borghi, L., Castelnuovo, G., Saita, E., Freda, M. F., Varallo, G., Franceschini, C., & Musetti, A. (2023). Dreaming or daydreaming during COVID-19 lockdown: A comparison between maladaptive and nonmaladaptive daydreamers. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10(4), 331–345. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000333
Merckelbach, H., Otgaar, H., & Lynn, S. J. (2022). Empirical research on fantasy proneness and its correlates 2000–2018: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9(1), 2–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000272
Miranda, R., & Mennin, D. S. (2007). Depression, generalized anxiety disorder, and certainty in pessimistic predictions about the future. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9063-4
Morewedge, C. K., & Norton, M. I. (2009). When dreaming is believing: The (motivated) interpretation of dreams. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(2), 249–264. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013264
Morina, N., Lancee, J., & Arntz, A. (2017). Imagery rescripting as a clinical intervention for aversive memories: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 55, 6–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.11.003
Nakata, A. C. G., Diniz, J. B., Torres, A. R., Mathis, M. A., Fossaluza, V., & Braganças, C. A. (2007). Level of insight and clinical features of obsessive-compulsive disorder with and without body dysmorphic disorder. CNS Spectrums, 12(4), 295–303. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852900021052
Nielsen, T. (2017). Microdream neurophenomenology. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2017(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/nix001
Nir, Y., & Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: From phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(2), 88–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001
Noreika, V., Canales-Johnson, A., Koh, J., Taylor, M., Massey, I., & Bekinschtein, T. A. (2015). Intrusions of a drowsy mind: Neural markers of phenomenological unpredictability. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 202. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00202
O’Connor, K. P., & Robillard, S. (1995). Inference processes in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Some clinical observations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(8), 887–896. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(95)00042-V
Oakley, D. A. (1999). Hypnosis and conversion hysteria: A unifying model. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 4(3), 243–265. https://doi.org/10.1080/135468099395954
Oren, E., Eitam, B., & Dar, R. (2019). Intentional binding and obsessive-compulsive tendencies: A dissociation between indirect and direct measures of the sense of agency. Journal of Obsessive Compulsive and Related Disorders, 20, 59–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.11.002
Oren, E., Friedmann, N., & Dar, R. (2016). Things happen: Individuals with high obsessive–compulsive tendencies omit agency in their spoken language. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 125–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.012
Pancheri, C., Caredda, M., Pacchiarotti, I., Chiaie, R. D., & Biondi, M. (2017). Dissociative identity disorder, self-hypnosis and psychotic-like symptoms: A problem of differential diagnosis. Rivista Di Psichiatria, 52(5), 208–211. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2017-54115-005
Patrick, A., & Durndell, A. (2004). Lucid dreaming and personality: A replication. Dreaming, 14(4), 234–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/1053-0797.14.4.234
Paulik, G., Newman-Taylor, K., Steel, C., & Arntz, A. (2022). Managing dissociation in imagery rescripting for voice hearers with trauma: Lessons from a case series. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 29(2), 434–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2020.06.009
Perogamvros, L., Baird, B., Seibold, M., Riedner, B., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2017). The phenomenal contents and neural correlates of spontaneous thoughts across wakefulness, NREM sleep, and REM sleep. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(10), 1766–1777. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01155
Perogamvros, L., & Schwartz, S. (2012). The roles of the reward system in sleep and dreaming. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(8), 1934–1951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.010
Pesant, N., & Zadra, A. (2006). Dream content and psychological well-being: A longitudinal study of the continuity hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 111–121. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20212
Pietkiewicz, I. J., Nęcki, S., Bańbura, A., & Tomalski, R. (2018). Maladaptive daydreaming as a new form of behavioral addiction. Journal of Behavioural Addictions, 7(3), 838–843. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.95
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83(2), 157–171. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.157
Pozza, A., Torniai, S., & Dèttore, D. (2016). Inferential confusion moderates the effects of dissociative experiences on OCD symptoms severity in a clinical sample with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 13(6), 108–111. Provost, J. (1999). A dream focus for short-term growth groups. Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 24(1), 74–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/01933929908411420
Rachman, S. (2007). Unwanted intrusive images in obsessive compulsive disorders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(4), 402–410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.008
Rao, R., & Ballard, D. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 79–87. https://doi.org/10.1038/4580
Rauschenberger, R. (2010). Reentrant processing in attentional guidance – time to abandon old dichotomies. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 109–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.014
Rauss, K., & Pourtois, G. (2013). What is bottom-up and what is top-down in predictive coding? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 276. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00276
Rechtschaffen, A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams. Sleep, 1, 97–109. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/1.1.97
Reznik, D., Gertner-Saad, L., Even-Furst, H., Henik, A., Ben Mair, E., Shechter-Amir, D., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Oneiric synesthesia: Preliminary evidence for the occurrence of synesthetic-like experiences during sleep-inertia. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5(4), 374–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000160
Ross, C. A. (2018). The potential relevance of maladaptive daydreaming in treatment of dissociative identity disorder in persons with ritual abuse and complex inner worlds. Frontiers in the Psychotherapy of Trauma and Dissociation, 1(2), 160–173. https://doi.org/10.XXXX/ftpd.2017.0010
Ross, C. A., Ellason, J. W., & Anderson, G. (1995). A factor analysis of the dissociative experiences scale (DES) in dissociative identity disorder. Dissociation, 8, 229–235. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1996-00775-004
Ross, C. A., Ridgway, J., & George, N. (2020). Maladaptive daydreaming, dissociation, and the dissociative disorders. Psychiatric Research & Clinical Practice, 2(2), 53–61. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.prcp.20190050
Salomon-Small, G., Somer, E., Harel-Schwarzmann, M., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2021). Maladaptive daydreaming and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: A confirmatory and exploratory investigation of shared mechanisms. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 136, 343–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.02.017
Savary, L. M. B., H., P., & Williams, S. K. (1984). Dreams and spiritual growth: A judeo-christian way of dreamwork. Paulist Press.
Scarpelli, S., Bartolacci, C., D’Atri, A., M, G., & Gennaro, L. (2019). The functional role of dreaming in emotional processes. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 459. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00459
Schädlich, M., Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2017). Improvement of darts performance following lucid dream practice depends on the number of distractions while rehearsing within the dream – a sleep laboratory pilot study. Journal of Sports Sciences, 35(23), 2365–2372. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1267387
Schimmenti, A., Sideli, L., Marca, L., Gori, A., & Terrone, G. (2020). Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the maladaptive daydreaming scale (MDS–16) in an italian sample. Journal of Personality Assessment, 102(5), 689–701. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2019.1594240
Schredl, M. (2003). Continuity between waking and dreaming: A proposal for a mathematical model. Sleep and Hypnosis, 5(1), 26–39. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2003-03507-005
Schredl, M. (2008). Laboratory references in dreams: Methodological problem and/or evidence for the continuity hypothesis of dreaming? International Journal of Dream Research, 1(1), 3–6. https://doi.org/10.11588/heidok.00008425
Schredl, M., Bohusch, C., Kahl, J., Mader, A., & Somesan, A. (2000). The use of dreams in psychotherapy: A survey of psychotherapists in private practice. The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 9(2), 81–87. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10793127/
Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2010). The continuity between waking mood and dream emotions: Direct and second-order effects. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 29(3), 271–282. https://doi.org/10.2190/IC.29.3.f
Schwartz, S. (2003). Are life episodes replayed during dreaming? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 (8), 325–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00162-1
Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., Schooler, J. W., & Smilek, D. (2018). Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Science, 22(6), 479–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010
Selvi, Y., Kiliç, S., Aydin, A., & Güzel Özdemir, P. (2015). The effects of sleep deprivation on dissociation and profiles of mood, and its association with biochemical changes. Archives of Neuropsychiatry, 52(1), 83–88. https://doi.org/10.5152/npa.2015.7116
Shafran, R., & Rachman, S. (2004). Thought-action fusion: A review. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35(2), 87–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.04.002
Shahar, G. (2015). Erosion: The psychopathology of self-criticism. Oxford University Press.
Siclari, F., Baird, B., Perogamvros, L., Bernardi, G., LaRocque, J. L., Riedner, B., Boly, M., Postle, B. R., & Tononi, G. (2017). The neural correlates of dreaming. Nature Neuroscience, 20, 872–878. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4545
Sikka, P. (2019). How to study dream experiences. In K. Valli & R. J. Hoss (Eds.), Dreams: Understanding biology, psychology, and culture (Vol. 1, pp. 153–166). Greenwood.
Smucker, M. R. (2005). Imagery rescripting and reprocessing therapy. In A. Freeman, S. H. Felgoise, C. M. Nezu, A. M. Nezu, & M. A. Reinecke (Eds.), Encyclopedia of cognitive behavior therapy (pp. 226–229). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-48581-8_63
Snyder, F. (1970). The phenomenology of dreaming. In L. Madow & L. H. Snow (Eds.), The psychodynamic implications of the physiological studies on dreams (pp. 124–151). C. Thomas.
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2014). Dissociation and dissociative mechanisms in panic disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and depression: A review and heuristic framework. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 243–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000023
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2017a). Arousal in nocturnal consciousness: How dream- and sleep-experiences may inform us of poor sleep quality, stress, and psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 733. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00733
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2017b). Daily elevations in dissociative absorption and depersonalization in a non-clinical sample are related to daily stress and psychopathological symptoms. Psychiatry-Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 80(3), 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.2016.1247622
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2019). Dissociative absorption, mind-wandering, and attention deficit symptoms: Associations with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 51–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12186
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2023). Obsessive-compulsive symptoms and dissociative experiences: Suggested underlying mechanisms and implications for science and practice. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1132800. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1132800
Soffer-Dudek, N., Lassri, D., Soffer-Dudek, N., & Shahar, G. (2015). Dissociative absorption: An empirically unique, clinically relevant, dissociative factor. Consciousness & Cognition, 36, 338–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.013
Soffer-Dudek, N., Shelef, L., Oz, I., Levkovsky, A., Erlich, I., & Gordon, S. (2017). Absorbed in sleep: Dissociative absorption as a predictor of sleepiness following sleep deprivation in two high-functioning samples. Consciousness and Cognition, 48, 161–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.11.009
Soffer-Dudek, N., & Somer, E. (2018). Trapped in a daydream: Daily elevations in maladaptive daydreaming are associated with daily psychopathological symptoms. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00194
Soffer-Dudek, N., & Somer, E. (2022). Maladaptive daydreaming is a dissociative disorder: Supporting evidence and theory. In M. J. Dorahy, S. N. Gold, & J. A. O’Neil (Eds.), Dissociation and the dissociative disorders: Past, present, future (2nd ed., pp. 547–559). Taylor & Francis.
Soffer-Dudek, N., Somer, E., Abu-Rayya, H. M., Metin, B., & Schimmenti, A. (2020). Different cultures, similar daydream addiction? An examination of the cross-cultural measurement equivalence of the maladaptive daydreaming scale. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 9(4), 1056–1067. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00080
Soffer-Dudek, N., Wertheim, R., & Shahar, G. (2011). Lucid dreaming and resilience in the face of exposure to terrorism. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 24(1), 125–128. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20601
Somer, E. (2002). Maladaptive daydreaming: A qualitative inquiry. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 32(2), 195–210. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020597026919
Somer, E. (2018). Maladaptive daydreaming: Ontological analysis, treatment rationale: A pilot case study. Frontiers in the Psychotherapy of Trauma and Dissociation, 2(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.46716/ftpd.2017.0006
Somer, E., Abu-Rayya, H. M., & Brenner, R. (2021). Childhood trauma and maladaptive daydreaming: Fantasy functions and themes in a multi-country sample. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 22(3), 288–303. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2020.1809599
Somer, E., Cardeña, E., Catelan, R. F., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2021). Reality shifting: Psychological features of an emergent online daydreaming culture. Current Psychology, 42, 11415–11427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02439-3
Somer, E., J., L., Jopp, D. S., & Bigelsen, J. (2016). Development and validation of the maladaptive daydreaming scale (MDS. Consciousness and Cognition, 39, 77–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.001
Somer, E., Soffer-Dudek, N., & Ross, C. A. (2017). The comorbidity of daydreaming disorder (maladaptive daydreaming. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 205(7), 525–530. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000685
Somer, E., Soffer-Dudek, N., Ross, C. A., & Halpern, N. (2017). Maladaptive daydreaming: Proposed diagnostic criteria and their assessment with a structured clinical interview. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 4(2), 176–189. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000114
Sparrow, G. S. (2014). Analyzing chronic dream ego responses in co-creative dream analysis. International Journal of Dream Research, 7(1), 33–38. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2014.1.12128
Speckens, A. E. M., Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., & Cuthbert, B. (2007). Imagery special issue: Intrusive images and memories of earlier adverse events in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(4), 411–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.09.004
Spitzer, C., Barnow, S., Freyberger, H. J., & Grabe, H. J. (2007). Dissociation predicts symptom-related treatment outcome in short-term inpatient psychotherapy. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 41(8), 682–687. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048670701449146
Spoormaker, V. I., & Bout, J. van den. (2006). Lucid dreaming treatment for nightmares: A pilot study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 75, 389–394. https://doi.org/10.1159/000095446
Spoormaker, V. I., Bout, J. van den, & Meijer, E. J. (2003). Lucid dreaming treatment for nightmares: A series of cases. Dreaming, 13, 181–186. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025325529560
Sterpenich, V., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Vandewalle, G., Darsaud, A., Balteau, E., Dang-Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., D’Argembeau, A., Gais, S., Rauchs, G., Schabus, M., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Collette, F., & Maquet, P. (2007). Sleep-related hippocampo- cortical interplay during emotional memory recollection. PLoS Biology, 5(11), 282. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050282
Sterpenich, V., Albouy, G., Darsaud, A., Schmidt, C., Vandewalle, G., Dang Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., Phillips, C., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Collette, F., Luxen, A., & Maquet, P. (2009). Sleep promotes the neural reorganization of remote emotional memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 5143–5152. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0561-09.2009
Strachan, L. P., Hyett, M. P., & McEvoy, P. M. (2020). Imagery rescripting for anxiety disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder: Recent advances and future directions. Current Psychiatry Reports, 22, 17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-1139-4
Theodor-Katz, N., Somer, E., Maaravi Hesseg, R., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2022). Could immersive daydreaming underlie a deficit in attention? The prevalence and characteristics of maladaptive daydreaming in individuals with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 78(11), 2309–2328. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23355
Van der Kloet, D., Merckelbach, H., Giesbrecht, T., & Lynn, S. J. (2012). Fragmented sleep, fragmented mind: The role of sleep in dissociative symptoms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(2), 159–175. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612437597
Van Eeden, F. (1913). A study of dreams. Proceedings of the Society for Psychical Research, 26, 413–461. http://dreamscience.ca/en/documents/New%20content/lucid%20dreaming%20pdfs/vanEeden_PSPR_26_1-12_1913.pdf
Van Heughten – van der Kloet, D., Giesbrecht, T., & Merckelbach, H. (2015). Sleep loss increases dissociation and affects memory for emotional stimuli. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 47, 9–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.11.002
Van Rijn, E., Eichenlaub, J.-B., Lewis, P. A., Walker, M. P., Gaskell, M. G., Malinowski, J. E., & Blagrove, M. (2015). The dream-lag effect: Selective processing of personally significant events during rapid eye movement sleep, but not during slow wave sleep. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 122, 98–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2015.01.009
Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Tuin, I., & Hobson, J. A. (2009). Lucid dreaming: A state of consciousness with features of both waking and non-lucid dreaming. Sleep, 32(9), 1191–1200. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/32.9.1191
Wamsley, E., Donjacour, C. E., Scammell, T. E., Lammers, G. J., & Stickgold, R. (2014). Delusional confusion of dreaming and reality in narcolepsy. Sleep, 37(2), 419–422. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.3428
Watson, D. (2001). Dissociations of the night: Individual differences in sleep-related experiences and their relation to dissociation and schizotypy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 526–535. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.110.4.526
Wen, H., Soffer-Dudek, N., & Somer, E. (2022). Daily feelings and the affective valence of daydreams in maladaptive day-dreaming: A longitudinal analysis. Psychology of Consciousness. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000293
West, M., & Somer, E. (2020). Empathy, emotion regulation and creativity in immersive daydreaming. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 39(4), 358–373. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276236619864277
Windt, J., & Voss, U. (2018). Spontaneous thought, insight, and control in lucid dreams. In K. Christoff & K. C. R. Fox (Eds.), The oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and dreaming (pp. 385–410). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.26
Wolman, R. N., & Kozmová, M. (2007). Last night i had the strangest dream: Varieties of rational thought processes in dream reports. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 838–849. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.009
Yu, C. K.-C. (2013). Obsessive-compulsive distress and its dynamic associations with schizotypy, borderline personality, and dreaming. Dreaming, 23(1), 46–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030791
Zadra, A. L., & Pihl, R. O. (1997). Lucid dreaming as a treatment for recurrent nightmares. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66, 50–55. https://doi.org/10.1159/000289106
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Copyright (c) 2024 Nirit Soffer-Dudek